ABSTRACT Title of Document: DETERMINANTS OF COMMUNAL STRENGTH: THE EFFECTS OF THE OTHER?S LIKEABILITY AND OF BENEFITING THE OTHER Ryan Dean Curtis, M.A., 2006 Directed By: Professor Judson Mills, Department of Psychology This research investigated potential determinants of communal strength. Communal strength refers to the degree of motivation to respond to a communal partner?s needs. It was hypothesized that increased liking for the other and giving a benefit to the other would each increase communal strength toward the other. Female participants completed pretest and posttest measures of communal strength toward another female student. Participants heard an audiotape that made the other sound likable or unlikable and in need of advice or not in need of advice. The participants gave advice to the other or observed someone else giving advice to the other. The results of an analysis of covariance of the posttest measure of communal strength, controlling for the pretest measure of communal strength, revealed support for both hypotheses. Communal strength toward the other increased when the other was likable, and when the person gave a benefit to the other. DETERMINANTS OF COMMUNAL STRENGTH: THE EFFECTS OF THE OTHER?S LIKEABILITY AND OF BENEFITING THE OTHER By Ryan Dean Curtis Thesis submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of the University of Maryland, College Park, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts 2006 Advisory Committee: Professor Judson Mills, Chair Professor Harold Sigall Professor Charles Stangor ? Copyright by Ryan Dean Curtis 2006 ii Dedication To my Heavenly Father and my wife Katie with whom I hope to always have the strongest of communal relationships. iii Table of Contents Dedication..................................................................................................................... ii Table of Contents......................................................................................................... iii List of Tables ............................................................................................................... iv List of Figures............................................................................................................... v Chapter 1: Introduction.................................................................................................1 Chapter 2: Method ...................................................................................................... 12 Chapter 3: Results....................................................................................................... 21 Chapter 4: Discussion ................................................................................................. 28 Appendix..................................................................................................................... 31 References................................................................................................................... 32 iv List of Tables Table 1. The 10-Item Communal Strength Scale Table 2. Means for Pretest Liking Table 3. Means for Posttest Liking Table 4. Means for Pretest Communal Strength Table 5. Means for Posttest Communal Strength Table 4. Adjusted Means for Posttest Liking Table 5. Adjusted Means for Posttest Communal Strength v List of Figures Figure 1. When a Communal Norm Applies 1 Chapter 1: Introduction For most people, their most important relationships are their communal relationships. Communal relationships are relationships in which benefits are given in response to needs or to demonstrate a general concern for the other person (Clark & Mills, 1979). Communal relationships can be contrasted with exchange relationships in which a person gives a benefit in expectation of receiving a benefit of equal value in return or as repayment for a previously received benefit. A benefit is ?something that one person chooses to give to another which is of use to the person receiving it,? (Clark & Mills, 1993, p. 687). For example, the relationship an auto mechanic has with a client is an exchange relationship. If the mechanic replaces a client?s clutch, he will expect adequate payment from that client. However, the mechanic may replace his daughter?s clutch and not expect payment for that service. This is because the mechanic has a communal relationship with his daughter. The benefit of the clutch replacement was given simply because the daughter had a need. Although a specific benefit is not expected in return for a benefit given in a communal relationship, this does not mean that communal relationships are not rewarding. When discussing rewards in communal relationships, it is important to distinguish rewards from benefits. As defined earlier, a benefit is something someone chooses to give that will be of use to the other. The fulfillment felt by the mechanic for changing his daughter?s clutch is not a benefit to him because it was not intentionally given by the daughter. The replacement of the clutch by the mechanic, however, was a benefit to the daughter and to the client, as was the money given by 2 the client to the mechanic. These are each benefits because each was intended to be useful to the other in the relationship (Clark & Mills, 1993). A communal relationship can vary quantitatively (Mills & Clark, 1982). Two important ways that communal relationships vary quantitatively are in certainty and communal strength. A person is generally much more certain of a communal relationship with an old friend or a spouse than with a stranger or new friend. The more certain a person is of the communal relationship, the less the person will be concerned that giving benefits to the other will be construed as desiring an exchange relationship as opposed to a communal relationship. Communal relationships also vary in communal strength. Communal strength refers to ?the degree of responsibility a person feels for a particular communal partner?s welfare? (Mills, Clark, Ford & Johnson, 2004, p. 213). The stronger the communal strength, the more responsibility felt for the other?s welfare. The stronger the communal strength, the more cost a person will incur to provide for the needs of the other. A person will feel more distress or guilt for not responding to the other?s need when communal strength is stronger. Within the hierarchy of communal relationships, communal strength will determine whose needs will or will not be met. If a person has two communal partners with the same need at the same time and only one partner?s need can be met by the person, then the communal partner towards whom the person has greater communal strength will receive the benefit. For an example of variation in communal strength, let us return to the example of the auto mechanic. The mechanic may help a neighbor change his oil and not expect specific compensation, but he would probably expect compensation to replace 3 his neighbor?s clutch. While the mechanic is willing to provide a costly benefit to one communal partner, he may not be willing to provide the same benefit to another communal partner with whom there is a weaker communal relationship. For the mechanic, the cost of replacing a clutch is quite high compared to the cost of an oil change. The mechanic?s communal strength toward his daughter is sufficient for him to replace her clutch simply to provide for her need, not expecting remuneration. However, the communal strength toward his neighbor is probably not sufficient to provide the benefit of replacing the neighbor?s clutch without payment for the service. It is, however, sufficient to help the neighbor change his oil without expectation of repayment, whereas the mechanic?s communal strength toward a stranger would probably not be sufficient to provide the benefit of helping with the oil change without repayment. Figure 1 shows how the communal norm will apply as a function of the communal relationship strength and the cost of the benefit. Need for the benefit can also affect when the benefit will be given without expectation of repayment. For example, the mechanic may not help a stranger put a new tire on his car without adequate payment, but he may help the stranger change a flat on the freeway for free. The benefit of changing the tire is essentially identical in both cases, however the need for the benefit is greater when it is a flat on the freeway. 4 Figure 1 When a Communal Norm Applies (from Clark & Mills, 1993) Communal strength is not the same as communal orientation (Clark, Oulette, Powell, & Milberg, 1987). Communal orientation refers to a person?s ?general tendency to follow communal norms in relationships? (Mills, Clark, Ford & Johnson, 2004, p. 216). It also takes into account the person?s expectation that others will follow a communal norm when interacting with the person. In contrast, communal strength refers to a person?s motivation to provide for the needs of a specific other. Mills, Clark, Ford, and Johnson (2004) created a measure of communal strength which is presented in Table 1. Items for the measure were chosen by having participants answer potential items regarding a best friend, acquaintance, immediate family member, and distant relative. Very High Very Low Low Stranger Friend Best Friend Child High Communal Norm Applies Cost of the Benefit Communal Relationship Strength 5 Table 1 The 10-Item Communal Strength Scale (Mills, Clark, Ford, & Johnson, 2004) How far would you be willing to go to visit ________? How happy would you feel when doing something that helps _____? How large a benefit would you be likely to give ________? How large a cost would you incur to meet a need of ______? How readily can you put the needs of ____ out of your mind? How high a priority for you is meeting the needs of ______? How reluctant would you be to sacrifice for ________? How much would you be willing to give up to benefit _____? How far would you go out of your way to do something for ____? How easily could you accept not helping ________? Note. Items 5, 7, and 10 are reverse scored. The instructions given are as follows: Keeping in mind the specific person, answer the following questions. As you answer each question, fill in the person?s initials in the blank. Circle one answer for each question on the scale from 0=not at all to 10=extremely before going on to the next question. Your answers will remain confidential. Responses to these items were found to be significantly different for immediate family members and distant relatives as well as for best friends and acquaintances. The ten items with the highest item-total correlations were included in the measure. Mills, et al. (2004) found that their communal strength measure had a 6 strong positive correlation with ratings of subjective closeness, as well as a strong positive correlation with participants? reports of the others? responsiveness to their needs. In order to show that communal strength is not the same as liking, Mills et al. (2004) had participants fill out the communal strength measure and a liking measure for a new, same-sex friend and a same-sex relative, each of approximately the same age as the participant. It was found that liking and communal strength correlated highly with each other for the new friend and also for the relative. As expected, communal strength was significantly higher for the relative than the new friend. Also as expected, liking was higher for the new friend than for the relative. This shows that, though correlated, liking and communal strength are distinct constructs. Mills et al. (2004) found that participants were more likely to give a benefit to someone with whom they had a stronger communal relationship. Participants also reported giving more help to, and receiving more help from, friends associated with a stronger communal relationship. In a study of married couples, the communal strength that the respondent indicated toward his or her spouse was strongly correlated with the marital satisfaction reported by the spouse. The husband?s communal strength toward his wife was significantly correlated with the marriage satisfaction reported by the wife even when controlling for the husband?s own communal orientation and marital satisfaction. Similarly, the wife?s communal strength toward her husband was correlated with the marriage satisfaction reported by the husband even when controlling for the wife?s own communal orientation and marital satisfaction. Mills, et al.?s (2004) studies of the communal strength measure 7 dealt with communal strength toward a previously acquainted other. However, the measure can also be used to measure the communal strength toward a new acquaintance. The purpose of the current study is to test some potential determinants of communal strength. One of these is the likeability of the other. As has been shown by Mills, et al. (2004), liking is correlated with communal strength. Generally, people have stronger communal relationships with people they like more. This could be because greater liking leads to an increase in the motivation to meet the other?s needs (Mills, et al. 2004). It could also be because increased communal strength leads to increased liking, or that liking and communal strength are both influenced by a third, common causal variable. In the present research, it was hypothesized that increased liking for the other would increase communal strength toward the other. Greater liking should increase communal strength because providing benefits to someone who is liked is rewarding. The more liked the other is, the more positive affect will result from providing for that person?s needs. The idea that a person who benefits a liked other will experience positive affect is not new. In one of the first social psychology textbooks, McDougall (1908) stated, ?as Shand points out, when a man has acquired the sentiment of love for a person or other object, he is apt to experience . . . joy when the object prospers.? (pg. 106). The more an individual likes another person, the more joy the individual should feel when that person is benefited. If a person experiences positive affect when the other is benefited, then that person should be more motivated to provide benefits to 8 the other. Since communal strength is the motivation to benefit the other when the other has a need, an increase in liking for the other should result in an increase in communal strength toward the other. There are factors that could moderate the effects of liking on communal strength. One of these is certainty about the relationship. People are generally very certain about strong communal relationships. This is because the person can be sure that giving benefits to the other is understood to be in fulfillment of a need (Mills & Clark, 1982). If certainty of the communal relationship is high, then changes in liking should have a limited effect on communal strength toward the other. For example, the certainty of a communal relationship toward a cousin is generally quite high even if the person is not well acquainted with the cousin. If the person happens to spend more time with the cousin and liking for the cousin increases, communal strength still may not increase much. This is because the certainty about the communal relationship was high before the increase in liking. The certainty of the strength of a communal relationship with a new acquaintance, however, is usually low. Changes in liking should have more of an effect on communal strength in these relationships because of low certainty about the relationship. A second potential determinant of communal strength is the giving of a benefit within a communal relationship. For the purposes of this study, a benefit is defined as something that is perceived by the giver to be of use or value to the other (Mills & Clark, 1982). Therefore, if what is given to the other is of no use to the other, then it is not truly a benefit. If a person gives something to another, but thinks 9 the other does not have any use for what is given, then the person should not perceive the action as giving a benefit. It was hypothesized in this research that giving a benefit to the other would increase the person?s communal strength toward the other. Giving a benefit to another could increase communal strength toward the other in two general ways. One is by affecting the person?s perception of the specific relationship. Another is by affecting the person?s self perception. Just as certainty of the communal relationship may moderate the effect of liking on communal strength, the effect of giving a benefit may also be moderated by certainty. Giving a benefit may change the person?s perception of the specific relationship by changing the person?s expectation of being liked by the other. The person may assume that the other will like the person more as a consequence of the giving of the benefit. The perception of being liked by the other will increase the person?s liking for the other (Berscheid & Reis, 1998). An increase in liking for the other may increase communal strength for the other, as discussed previously. Another way providing a benefit could affect the person?s perception of the relationship is through a perceived increase in communal strength of the other toward the person. The fact that the person gave the other a benefit in a communal relationship may lead the person to assume that the communal strength of the other toward the person will be greater. The perception of greater communal strength of the other toward the person could increase the person?s communal strength toward the other. Mills and Clark (2001) have suggested that it is a cultural norm for romantic partners to be equal in communal strength. This norm should also apply to friends 10 and acquaintances such that an increase in the other?s communal strength toward the person could result in an increase of the person?s communal strength toward the other. Giving a benefit may change the person?s perception of the relationship by making salient that the person follows the communal norm in relation to the specific other. Being aware that one follows the communal norm regarding the other could increase the communal strength of the person toward the other. The other general way that giving a benefit could affect communal strength is through a change in the person?s self perception. When the person provides a benefit to the other, the person?s perception of their own communal orientation may increase. As discussed previously, communal orientation is a person?s general tendency to follow the communal norm. When the person provides a benefit to another in a communal relationship, the person?s general tendency to follow the communal norm may become more salient. This increase in communal orientation could increase communal strength toward the specific other, and it could also increase communal strength toward someone who did not receive the benefit. The present research investigated communal strength toward another person with whom the participant was not previously acquainted. Participants filled out a pre and posttest communal strength measure regarding another student. Likeability for the other was manipulated by having the participant hear the other answer questions in a manner designed to make the other appear likable or unlikable. In some conditions, the participant gave advice to the other and in other conditions the participant did not give advice, but heard someone else give advice to the other. 11 Sometimes the other indicated a need for advice and sometimes not. It was hypothesized that communal strength would be greater toward the likeable other. It was also hypothesized that communal strength toward the other would be greater when the participant gave advice and the other needed advice. 12 Chapter 2: Method Overview Female participants were assigned to either an advisor or observer condition. In the advisor condition, the participant listened to an audiotape of another female student (the advisee), which made the advisee appear likable or unlikable and in need of advice or not. The participant then recorded (scripted) advice to the advisee. In the observer condition, the participant heard the same audiotape by the advisee and listened to an audiotape of another participant giving the advice to the advisee. In both the advisor and observer conditions, the participant completed a communal strength measure and a liking measure regarding the advisee both before hearing the advisee?s tape and after giving (or hearing) advice to the advisee. Participants The participants were 80 female introductory psychology students at the University of Maryland who received extra credit for their participation. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of eight conditions in the 2 (likable/unlikable) x 2 (advisor/observer) x 2 (high need/low need) between-subjects design. Procedure Participants signed up for an experiment entitled ?Communication Among Strangers.? Upon arrival at the lab, the participant was greeted by the experimenter and led into a lab room where she was seated at a table on which there was a tape recorder, a digital camera, and a tape player. After filling out a consent form, the 13 experimenter explained that the purpose of the study was to see if the communication media makes a difference when strangers talk about personal information, and that the current experiment would be studying communication via audiotape. Advisor Condition Participants in the advisor condition were told that there were two participant conditions in the experiment, advisee and advisor. The advisee talks about a topic that is personally relevant to her and the advisor gives advice about that topic. The participant was told that she had been assigned to the advisor condition and that another student who had previously been assigned to the advisee condition was asked to talk about choosing a major. The participant was informed that she would hear an audiotape of the advisee talking about choosing a major and then be recorded giving advice to the advisee. The advisee would hear the tape of the advice at a future time and give her reactions. The participant would be asked to predict how the advisee will react when she hears the advice. The participant was then told that she would see a photograph of the advisee to make sure that the advisee was a stranger to her. She was shown a photograph on the digital camera?s display of a 24 year old, female, Caucasian student. None of the participants indicated that they knew that person. The participant was then told that it was important for the experimenter to control for first impressions. To do this, the participant would fill out a questionnaire about the advisee. The participant was then given the ?First Impressions? questionnaire which consisted of 13 questions. The first ten questions on this questionnaire were the communal strength measure (see Table 1). The final three 14 items were the same liking measure used by Mills, et al. (2004). The liking measure used the same response format as the communal strength measure. The questions in the liking measure were: ?How much do you personally like ________??, ?How annoying do you find ________??, and ?How positive is your general evaluation of ________?? Each one of the questions on the questionnaire had a blank for the advisee?s initials previously filled in by the experimenter with the initials CS. The participant was told to answer the questions about the advisee whose initials, CS, had been previously filled in by the experimenter. When the participant finished with the questionnaire, the experimenter told her that some people find this questionnaire a little difficult to fill out at first, so the questionnaire would be given to her again to fill out later, and the two administrations would then be combined to get a more reliable measure. Before hearing the advisee?s tape, the participant was told that, to get the advisee used to talking about personal information on an audiotape, the first thing she would hear was the advisee responding to a questionnaire. The experimenter then handed the participant a blank questionnaire (see the Appendix). The questionnaire included nine statements to be responded to with one of 7 answers ranging from ?Agree Strongly,? to ?Disagree Strongly.? The participant was told that the questionnaire was only for her reference and that she did not need to fill out anything on this questionnaire. She was then told that, when the advisee finished responding to the questionnaire, she would immediately begin talking in her own words about choosing a major. The participant then listened, on headphones, to one of four 15 audiotapes, all recorded by the same person. Each tape represented one of the four conditions: Likable/High Need, Likable/Low Need, Unlikable/High Need, and Unlikable/Low Need. The first part of the tape consisted of the advisee?s responses to the statements on the questionnaire designed to make her seem likable or unlikable. In the Likeable Condition, the advisee responded as follows. ?Hi, my initials are C. S. Item 1. I like to make new friends. Agree strongly. Item 2. I lose my temper easily. Disagree strongly. Item 3. I don?t like to criticize people. Agree strongly. Item 4. I always let people know when they annoy me. Disagree strongly. Item 5. I don?t mind apologizing when I have offended someone. Agree strongly. Item 6. I try to go out of my way to cheer up someone who is feeling depressed. Agree strongly. Item 7. I don?t mind taking the blame for something done by a friend. Agree strongly. Item 8. I don?t like people who disagree with me. Disagree moderately. Item 9. When people criticize me, I try to put them on the defensive, by pointing out their faults. Disagree moderately.? In the Unlikable Condition, the advisee responded as follows. ?Hi, my initials are C. S. Item 1. I like to make new friends. Disagree moderately. Item 2. I lose my temper easily. Agree moderately. 16 Item 3. I don?t like to criticize people. Disagree moderately. Item 4. I always let people know when they annoy me. Agree strongly. Item 5. I don?t mind apologizing when I have offended someone. Disagree moderately. Item 6. I try to go out of my way to cheer up someone who is feeling depressed. Disagree slightly. Item 7. I don?t mind taking the blame for something done by a friend. Disagree slightly. Item 8. I don?t like people who disagree with me. Agree moderately. Item 9. When people criticize me, I try to put them on the defensive, by pointing out their faults. Agree moderately.? On the second part of the tape, the advisee talked about choosing a major. The advisee?s need for help was manipulated by altering four sentences in the script. The following is what the advisee said in the High Need Condition. ?Um, I guess I?m supposed to talk a little bit about choosing a major. This is actually something that I?ve been thinking a lot about lately. I really need a lot of help choosing a major. I don?t know what to do at all and I?m really worried about it. I have to decide soon, though ?cuz my parents keep asking me about it. Right now, I?m trying to decide between a couple of majors. I really like both of them and I need to choose between the two. I?ve taken classes in both subjects and I?ve noticed that there are some things that I really love and there are some things that I really don?t like about both of them. It?s been good taking the different classes ?cuz I?ve seen what the major would 17 really be like, but there are pros and cons for each of them. I?m really worried about actually declaring a major. I feel like I don?t know a lot of people who can help me decide.? In the Low Need Condition, the participant heard the following. ?Um, I guess I?m supposed to talk a little bit about choosing a major. This is actually something that I?ve been thinking a lot about lately. I don?t think I need a lot of help necessarily. I think I know what to do, it?s just a matter of making a decision. I have to decide soon, though ?cuz my parents keep asking me about it. Right now, I?m trying to decide between a couple of majors. I really like both of them and I need to choose between the two. I?ve taken classes in both subjects and I?ve noticed that there are some things that I really love and there are some things that I really don?t like about both of them. It?s been good taking the different classes ?cuz I?ve seen what the major would really be like, but there are pros and cons for each of them. I?m not really worried about actually declaring a major. I feel like I know a lot of people who can help me decide.? After the participant heard the audiotape, the experimenter told her to remove the headphones and collected the questionnaire from her. The participant was then told that she was now ready to give her advice to the advisee. She was told that, since this was a study of the communication media, it was important to control for the content of her advice to the advisee. This meant that, even though the advisee spoke in her own words, she would be given a script to use. She was told that this was so all of the advisees receive the same advice and thus their reactions could be compared equally. 18 The experimenter then gave the participant a copy of the advisor?s script which is as follows. ?I am going to give you some advice about choosing a major. Choosing a major is an important decision. There are a few places where you can go to get specific advice. One is the career counseling center. There, you can talk to someone who can give you advice about choosing a major. They can also help you choose your major depending on what you want to do for your future career. They have a variety of tests that you can take that will help clarify what your interests are. You may find something that you would be good at that you never thought of before. An important thing to remember is that you can always change your mind. After you?ve really thought about the different majors you?re interested in, and you?re leaning towards one, go ahead and declare it to be your major. Don?t completely get rid of your other interests, though. Keep taking classes in other things that you are interested in. Tell yourself that every semester, or every year, you are going to look at where you are in your major, and your other interests, and decide if you really want to stay with it. You will soon find out where you fit and don?t fit, and you can always keep your other interests as a minor. Above all, don?t worry about it too much. Just keep working at it and you will find something that?s right for you.? The participant was instructed to read through the script on her own for a few minutes. When she had done this, she was asked to read the script aloud once for 19 practice. She was told that the advice would only be recorded once, therefore it was important for her to make her reading sound as natural as possible. The experimenter then turned on the tape recorder, stated the participant?s number, and told the participant to begin. The participant then read the scripted advice to the advisee. When she was finished, the experimenter pressed stop on the recorder and collected the script. The participant was then reminded that she would be given the first impressions questionnaire again and that it was to be filled out about the advisee. She then filled out the ?first impressions? questionnaire consisting of the communal strength and liking measures. After the questionnaire was completed, the experimenter checked the participant for suspicion by telling her that there was more to the study than she was initially told. She was asked if she had an idea as to what that might be. Eleven students were not included as participants in the study because they suspected that the advisee was scripted. No participant suspected the true hypothesis of the study. The experimenter explained why all of the information about the experiment may not be given to a participant at the beginning of a study. The participant was told that the experimenter was attempting to create a situation where he could get at the spontaneous and natural reactions of the participant. She was then told the true purpose of the experiment and asked not to divulge it. After any questions were thoroughly answered, the participant was excused. 20 Observer Condition The procedure for the observer condition was identical to the procedure in the advisor condition except that, in the observer condition, the participant was told that there are three participant conditions; an advisee, an advisor, and an observer. She was told that she has been assigned to the observer condition, while other students had previously been assigned to the advisee and advisor conditions. The advisee and advisor roles were explained as in the advisor condition. The participant was told that, as the observer, she would listen to the advisee?s tape and the advisor?s advice and predict how the advisee will react when she hears the advice. The participant then saw the photograph of the advisee and filled out the communal strength and liking measures exactly as in the advisor condition. The participant also heard the advisee?s tape exactly as in the advisor condition. After hearing the advisee?s tape, the participant was told that she would hear the tape of the advisor giving advice to the advisee. She was told that the advisor used a script in order to control for the content of the advice given to the advisee. The participant was then given the script and listened to the most recently recorded tape of a participant in the advisor condition reading the advice. The participant then filled out the communal strength and liking measures again, as in the advisor condition. The participant was then checked for suspicion and debriefed as in the advisor condition. 21 Chapter 3: Results Scores for the two administrations of the liking measure were calculated separately for each participant by averaging the responses to the three items after Item 2 was reverse scored. Means for the pretest and posttest measure of liking are presented in Tables 2 and 3 respectively. Table 2 Means for Pretest Liking Participant?s Role Other?s Need Other?s Likeability Advisor Observer High High 6.20 6.43 High Low 6.03 5.83 Low High 6.17 6.63 Low Low 6.30 5.70 Both High 6.18 6.53 Both Low 6.17 5.77 High Both 6.12 6.13 Low Both 6.23 6.17 Both Both 6.18 6.15 Note. n=10 in each of the eight conditions 22 Table 3 Means for Posttest Liking Participant?s Role Other?s Need Other?s Likeability Advisor Observer High High 7.03 7.13 High Low 4.20 3.93 Low High 6.40 6.27 Low Low 4.13 3.13 Both High 6.72 6.70 Both Low 4.17 3.53 High Both 5.62 5.53 Low Both 5.27 4.70 Both Both 5.44 5.12 Note. n=10 in each of the eight conditions Cronbach?s Alpha was .32 for the pretest and .89 for the posttest. Scores for the pretest and posttest were positively correlated (r = .31, p < .01). 23 The two administrations of the communal strength measure were scored for each participant by averaging the responses to each of the ten items after Items 5, 7, and 10 were reverse scored. Means for the pretest and posttest measure of communal strength are presented in Tables 4 and 5 respectively. Table 4 Means for Pretest Communal Strength Participant?s Role Other?s Need Other?s Likeability Advisor Observer High High 4.68 4.00 High Low 4.41 4.40 Low High 5.07 4.75 Low Low 4.33 3.68 Both High 4.88 4.38 Both Low 4.37 4.04 High Both 4.55 4.20 Low Both 4.70 4.22 Both Both 4.62 4.21 Note. n=10 in each of the eight conditions 24 Table 5 Means for Posttest Communal Strength Participant?s Role Other?s Need Other?s Likeability Advisor Observer High High 6.00 4.50 High Low 4.22 2.85 Low High 5.23 5.17 Low Low 3.28 2.29 Both High 5.62 4.84 Both Low 3.75 2.57 High Both 5.11 3.68 Low Both 4.25 3.73 Both Both 4.68 3.70 Note. n=10 in each of the eight conditions Cronbach?s Alpha was .85 for the pretest and .91 for the posttest. Scores for the pretest and posttest were positively correlated (r = .63, p < .01). The pretest measure of liking and the pretest measure of communal strength were positively correlated (r = .43, p < .01). The posttest measure of liking and the posttest measure of communal strength were highly positively correlated (r = .74, p < 25 .01). These results replicate the findings of Mills, et al. (2004) that liking and communal strength are significantly correlated. In order to check on the manipulation of likeability, a 2 (likable/unlikable) x 2 (advisor/observer) x 2 (high need/low need) analysis of covariance was calculated on the posttest liking measure, with the pretest measure of liking as a covariate. The adjusted means for all eight conditions are presented in Table 6. Table 6 Adjusted Means for Posttest Liking Participant?s Role Other?s Need Other?s Likeability Advisor Observer High High 7.02 7.03 High Low 4.25 4.07 Low High 6.40 6.08 Low Low 4.08 3.32 Both High 6.71 6.55 Both Low 4.17 3.69 High Both 5.64 5.55 Low Both 5.24 4.70 Both Both 5.44 5.12 Note. n=10 in each of the eight conditions 26 The main effect for likeability was the only statistically significant effect, F(1,79) = 40.88, p < .01, MSE = 3.71. The posttest adjusted mean of liking was 6.63 in the likable condition and 3.93 in the unlikable condition. These results indicate that the manipulation of likeability was successful. In order to test the hypotheses, a 2 (likable/unlikable) x 2 (advisor/observer) x 2 (high need/low need) analysis of covariance was conducted on the posttest communal strength measure, with the pretest measure of communal strength as a covariate. The main effect of likeability was significant, F,(1,79) = 49.24, p < .01, MSE = 1.24. The adjusted posttest communal strength mean was higher in the likable condition (5.08) than in the unlikable condition (3.31). The analysis of covariance also revealed a significant main effect for advisor/observer, F(1,79) = 7.40, p < .01. This main effect was qualified by a significant interaction between advisor/observer and need, F(1,79) = 4.12, p < .05. There were no other significant results. The adjusted means for all eight conditions are presented in Table 7. The adjusted posttest for communal strength of 5.02 for the advisor/high need condition was significantly higher than the adjusted posttest for communal strength of 4.05 for the advisor/low need condition, (t = 2.04, p < .01). The adjusted posttest mean for the advisor/high need condition was also significantly higher than the adjusted posttest of 3.83 for the observer/high need condition, (t = 2.51, p < .01). 27 Table 7 Adjusted Means for Posttest Communal Strength Participant?s Role Other?s Need Other?s Likeability Advisor Observer High High 5.81 4.80 High Low 4.22 2.86 Low High 4.76 4.93 Low Low 3.34 2.81 Both High 5.29 4.86 Both Low 3.78 2.84 High Both 5.02 3.83 Low Both 4.05 3.87 Both Both 4.54 3.85 Note. n=10 in each of the eight conditions There was no significant difference between the adjusted posttest for the advisor/low need and the observer/low need conditions, (t = .38). There was also no significant difference between the adjusted posttest for the observer/high need and the observer/low need conditions, (t = .10). 28 Chapter 4: Discussion The results support both of the hypotheses concerning determinants of communal strength. The hypothesis that increased liking for the other increases communal strength was supported by the significant main effect of likeability on the posttest measure of communal strength. Adjusted posttest communal strength was higher for the likable other than the unlikable other. Communal strength was greater for the likable other in the advisor condition and also in observer condition. It was also greater for the likable other in the high need condition as well as in the low need condition. Increased liking for the other increased communal strength toward the other regardless of whether or not a benefit was given by the person and regardless of the other?s need. The hypothesis that increased liking would increase communal strength was based on the idea that providing benefits within communal relationships is rewarding. The person should expect to experience more positive affect from benefiting the other the more the other is liked. Communal strength should increase because the more rewarding it is to give benefits to the other, the greater should be the motivation to provide benefits to the other. The hypothesis that giving a benefit to the other would increase communal strength was supported by the significant interaction between advisor/observer and need. Participants who gave advice to the other who needed the advice had significantly higher adjusted posttest communal strength toward the other than did those who gave advice to the other who did not need the advice, as well as those who did not give advice to the other. Adjusted posttest communal strength was not 29 significantly different for participants who gave advice to the other who did not need the advice and participants who did not give advice to the other. That the advisee would be perceived by the advisor as receiving a benefit when need was high but not when need was low is based on the definition of a benefit. As discussed previously, a benefit is defined as something that will be of use to the other. It was assumed that participants who advised another student who did not need advice would not see the giving of advice as a benefit because the other had little or no use for what was given. Of course, in the observer condition the participant did not give a benefit because nothing was given. As mentioned in the introduction, giving a benefit could affect communal strength in several ways. One of these is through an effect on liking. Giving a benefit could lead the person to assume that the other likes the person more which could increase liking for the other and thus communal strength. However, the results for the liking measure indicate that giving a benefit did not increase communal strength by affecting liking. For the liking measure, there was no significant main effect of advisor/observer or interaction of advisor/observer with need. The evidence from the liking measure indicates that giving a benefit to the other did not increase liking for the other. Another way the person?s perception of the relationship could be affected by giving a benefit is that the person could assume that the other?s communal strength toward the person has increased, which could lead to an increase in communal strength of the person toward the other. Yet another way is that giving a benefit to the other could make the giving of benefits within the specific relationship more 30 salient. The present study does not provide evidence which could support or refute these possibilities. Giving a benefit could affect communal strength by changing the person?s self perception. Giving a benefit could make the person?s general tendency to follow a communal norm more salient, which could increase the person?s communal orientation. An increase in communal orientation could increase communal strength toward the other who was benefited and also could increase communal strength toward others who the person did not benefit. The results of this study also do not provide evidence that could support or refute this possibility. Understanding the determinants of communal strength is important because communal strength can have a significant impact on people?s relationships. Mills, et al. (2004) have shown that communal strength predicted allocation of benefits to others. It also predicted giving help to others and receiving help from others. Communal strength even predicted the marital satisfaction of a person?s spouse beyond the person?s own communal orientation and marital satisfaction. The present research advances the understanding of communal strength by providing evidence that the likeability of the other and the giving of a benefit to the other are both determinants of communal strength. 31 Appendix Your Initials _________ Please answer each of the following questions about yourself as honestly as possible. Answer each question with one of the following answers: Strongly agree, Moderately agree, Slightly agree, Neither agree nor disagree, Slightly disagree, Moderately disagree, Strongly disagree Item 1. I like to make new friends. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 2. I loose my temper easily. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 3. I don?t like to criticize people. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 4. I always let people know when they annoy me. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 5. I don?t mind apologizing when I have offended someone. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 6. I try to go out of my way to cheer up someone who is feeling depressed. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 7. I don?t mind taking the blame for something done by a friend. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 8. I don?t like people who disagree with me. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly Item 9. When people criticize me, I try to put them on the defensive, by pointing out their faults. Agree Strongly Agree Moderately Agree Slightly Neither Agree nor Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Moderately Disagree Strongly 32 References Berscheid, E., & Reis, H. T. (1998). Attraction and close relationships. In: D. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology. (4 th ed.). (Vol. 2, pp. 193-281). New York: McGraw-Hill. Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal Attraction in exchange and communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1). 12-24. Clark, M S. & Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 19(6) 684-691. Clark, M. S., Ouellette, R., Powell, M., & Milberg, S. (1987). Relationship type, recipient mood, and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 94-103. McDougall, W. (1960). An Introduction to Social Psychology. London: Methuen. (Original work published 1908). Mills, J., & Clark, M. S. (1982). Exchange and communal relationships. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology, (Vol. 3, pp. 121- 144). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Mills, J., & Clark, M. S. (2001). Viewing close romantic relationships as communal relationships: Implications for maintenance and enhancement. In J. Harvey & A. Wenzel (Eds.), Close Romantic Relationships: Maintenance and Enhancement, (pp. 13-25). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Mills, J., Clark, M. S., Ford, T. E., & Johnson, M. (2004). Measurement of communal strength. Personal Relationships. 11, 213-230.